References

Batt LS, Batt MS, Baguley JA, McGreevy PD. Factors associated with success in guide dog training. J Vet Behav. 2008; 3:(4)143-151 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2008.04.003

Blackwell EJ, Bradshaw JWS, Casey RA. Fear responses to noises in domestic dogs: prevalence, risk factors and co-occurrence with other fear related behaviour. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2013; 145:(1–2)15-25 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.12.004

Bovenkerk B, Nijland HJ. The pedigree dog breeding debate in ethics and practice: beyond welfare arguments. J Agri Environ Ethics. 2017; 30:(3)387-412 https://doi.org/10.1007/s10806-017-9673-8

Boyd C, Jarvis S, McGreevy P Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged under three years attending primary-care veterinary practices in England. Anim Welf. 2018; 27:(3)251-262 https://doi.org/10.7120/09627286.27.3.251

Cannas S, Talamonti Z, Mazzola S, Minero M, Picciolini A, Palestrini C. Factors associated with dog behavioral problems referred to a behavior clinic. J Vet Behav. 2018; 24:42-47 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2017.12.004

Caron-Lormier G, England GCW, Green MJ, Asher L. Using the incidence and impact of health conditions in guide dogs to investigate healthy ageing in working dogs. Vet J. 2016; 207:124-130 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.tvjl.2015.10.046

Dale AR, Walker JK, Farnworth MJ, Morrissey SV, Waran NK. A survey of owners' perceptions of fear of fireworks in a sample of dogs and cats in New Zealand. N Z Vet J. 2010; 58:(6)286-291 https://doi.org/10.1080/00480169.2010.69403

Dodman NH, Karlsson EK, Moon-Fanelli A A canine chromosome 7 locus confers compulsive disorder susceptibility. Mol Psychiatry. 2010; 15:(1)8-10 https://doi.org/10.1038/mp.2009.111

Dreschel NA. The effects of fear and anxiety on health and lifespan in pet dogs. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2010; 3-4 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2010.04.003

Erhardt A, Spoormaker VI. Translational approaches to anxiety: focus on genetics, fear extinction and brain imaging. Curr Psychiatry Rep. 2013; 15:(12) https://doi.org/10.1007/s11920-013-0417-9

Evans RI, Herbold JR, Bradshaw BS, Moore GE. Causes for discharge of military working dogs from service: 268 cases (2000–2004). J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2007; 231:(8)1215-1220 https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.231.8.1215

Goddard ME, Beilharz RG. Genetics of traits which determine the suitability of dogs as guide-dogs for the blind. Appl Anim Ethol. 1983; 9:(3–4)299-315 https://doi.org/10.1016/0304-3762(83)90010-X

Handegård KW, Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Noise reactivity in standard poodles and irish soft-coated wheaten terriers. J Vet Behav. 2020; 36:(March–April)4-12 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2020.01.002

Handegård KW, Madsen P, Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Genetic parameters for noise reactivity in standard poodles. J Vet Behav. 2021; 45:33-36 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2021.06.005

Hsu Y, Serpell JA. Development and validation of a questionnaire for measuring behavior and temperament traits in pet dogs. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2003; 223:(9)1293-1300 https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.2003.223.1293

Jones AC, Gosling SD. Temperament and personality in dogs (canis familiaris): a review and evaluation of past research. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2005; 95:(1–2)1-53 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2005.04.008

Kwan JY, Bain MJ. Owner attachment and problem behaviors related to relinquishment and training techniques of dogs. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2013; 16:(2)168-183 https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2013.768923

Lazarowski L, Rogers B, Krichbaum S, Haney P, Smith JG, Waggoner P. Validation of a behavior test for predicting puppies' suitability as detection dogs. Animals (Basel). 2021; 11:(4) https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11040993

Lindberg S, Strandberg E, Swenson L. Genetic analysis of hunting behaviour in Swedish flatcoated retrievers. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2004; 88:(3–4)289-298 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2004.03.007

Lopes Fagundes AL, Hewison L, McPeake KJ, Zulch H, Mills DS. Noise sensitivities in dogs: an exploration of signs in dogs with and without musculoskeletal pain using qualitative content analysis. Front Vet Sci. 2018; 5 https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2018.00017

Marston LC, Bennett PC, Coleman GJ. What happens to shelter dogs? An analysis of data for 1 year from three Australian shelters. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2004; 7:(1)27-47 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0701_2

McFarland D. The Oxford companion to animal behaviour.Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1981

Meyer F, Schawalder P, Gaillard C, Dolf G. Estimation of genetic parameters for behavior based on results of German Shepherd dogs in Switzerland. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2012; 140:(1)53-61 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2012.05.007

Mobbs D, Adolphs R, Fanselow MS Viewpoints: approaches to defining and investigating fear. Nat Neurosci. 2019; 22:(8)1205-1216 https://doi.org/10.1038/s41593-019-0456-6

Morrow M, Ottobre J, Ottobre A Breed-dependent differences in the onset of fear-related avoidance behavior in puppies. J Vet Behav. 2015; 10:(4)286-294 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2015.03.002

Overall KL. Manual of clinical behavioural medicine for dogs and cats.St. Louis: Elsevier; 2013

Overall KL, Dunham AE, Frank D. Frequency of nonspecific clinical signs in dogs with separation anxiety, thunderstorm phobia, and noise phobia, alone or in combination. J Am Vet Med Assoc. 2001; 219:(4)467-473 https://doi.org/10.2460/javma.2001.219.467

Overall KL, Dunham AE, Juarbe-Diaz SV. Phenotypic determination of noise reactivity in 3 breeds of working dogs: a cautionary tale of age, breed, behavioral assessment, and genetics. J Vet Behav. 2016; 16:(November–December)113-125 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.09.007

Persson ME, Wright D, Roth LS, Batakis P, Jensen P. Genomic regions associated with interspecies communication in dogs contain genes related to human social disorders. Sci Rep. 2016; 6 https://doi.org/10.1038/srep33439

Ressler KJ. Translating across circuits and genetics toward progress in fear-and anxiety-related disorders. Am J Psychiatry. 2020; 177:(3)214-222 https://doi.org/10.1176/appi.ajp.2020.20010055

Riemer S. Not a one-way road – severity, progression and prevention of firework fears in dogs. PLoS One. 2019; 14:(9) https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0218150

Ruefenacht S, Gebhardt-Henrich S, Miyake T, Gaillard C. A behaviour test on German Shepherd dogs: heritability of seven different traits. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2002; 79:(2)113-132 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(02)00134-X

Rugbjerg H, Proschowsky HF, Ersbøll AK, Lund JD. Risk factors associated with interdog aggression and shooting phobias among purebred dogs in Denmark. Prev Vet Med. 2003; 58:(1–2)85-100 https://doi.org/10.1016/s0167-5877(03)00011-4

Salonen M, Sulkama S, Mikkola S Prevalence, comorbidity, and breed differences in canine anxiety in 13,700 Finnish pet dogs. Sci Rep. 2020; 10:(1) https://doi.org/10.1038/s41598-020-59837-z

Salonen M, Mikkola S, Hakanen E, Sulkama S, Puurunen J, Lohi H. Personality traits associated with behavioral problems in pet dogs. Transl Psychiatry. 2022; 12:(1) https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-022-01841-0

Sarviaho R, Hakosalo O, Tiira K Two novel genomic regions associated with fearfulness in dogs overlap human neuropsychiatric loci. Transl Psychiatry. 2019; 9:(1) https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-018-0361-x

Sarviaho R, Hakosalo O, Tiira K A novel genomic region on chromosome 11 associated with fearfulness in dogs. Transl Psychiatry. 2020; 10:(1) https://doi.org/10.1038/s41398-020-0849-z

Scarlett JM, Salman MD, New JG, Kass PH. Reasons for relinquishment of companion animals in U.S. animal shelters: selected health and personal issues. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 1999; 2:(1)41-57 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0201_4

Scheifele PM, Sonstrom KE, Dunham AE, Overall KL. Is noise reactivity reflected in auditory response variables, including those that measure cognition, in dogs? Initial findings. J Vet Behav. 2016; 16:(November–December)65-75 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.09.002

Estimation of genetic parameters for behavioral assessment scores in Labrador retrievers, German shepherd dogs, and golden retrievers. 2012. https//krex.k-state.edu/handle/2097/13660 (accessed 8 November 2023)

Shan S, Xu F, Brenig B. Genome-wide association studies reveal neurological genes for dog herding, predation, temperament, and trainability traits. Front Vet Sci. 2021; 8 https://doi.org/10.3389/fvets.2021.693290

Sherman BL, Mills DS. Canine anxieties and phobias: an update on separation anxiety and noise aversions. Vet Clin North Am Small Anim Pract. 2008; 38:(5)1081-vii https://doi.org/10.1016/j.cvsm.2008.04.012

Shore ER. Returning a recently adopted companion animal: adopters' reasons for and reactions to the failed adoption experience. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2005; 8:(3)187-198 https://doi.org/10.1207/s15327604jaws0803_3

Spady TC, Ostrander EA. Canine behavioral genetics: pointing out the phenotypes and herding up the genes. Am J Hum Genet. 2008; 82:(1)10-18 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.ajhg.2007.12.001

Stephens-Lewis D, Johnson A, Turley N, Naydorf-Hannis R, Scurlock-Evans L, Schenke KC. Understanding canine ‘reactivity’: species-specific behaviour or human inconvenience?. J Appl Anim Welf Sci. 2022; 1-15 https://doi.org/10.1080/10888705.2022.2147007

Storengen LM, Lingaas F. Noise sensitivity in 17 dog breeds: prevalence, breed risk and correlation with fear in other situations. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 2015; 171:152-160 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.applanim.2015.08.020

Strandberg E, Jacobsson J, Saetre P. Direct genetic, maternal and litter effects on behaviour in German Shepherd dogs in Sweden. Livest Prod Sci. 2005; 93:(1)33-42 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.livprodsci.2004.11.004

Tang R, Noh HJ, Wang D Candidate genes and functional noncoding variants identified in a canine model of obsessive-compulsive disorder. Genome Biol. 2014; 15:(3) https://doi.org/10.1186/gb-2014-15-3-r25

Tiira K, Lohi H. Early life experiences and exercise associated with canine anxieties. PLoS One. 2015; 10:(11) https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0141907

Tiira K, Sulkama S, Lohi H. Prevalence, comorbidity, and behavioral variation in canine anxiety. J Vet Behav. 2016; 16:(November–December)36-44 https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jveb.2016.06.008

van der Waaij EH, Wilsson E, Strandberg E. Genetic analysis of results of a Swedish behavior test on German Shepherd Dogs and Labrador Retrievers. J Anim Sci. 2008; 86:(11)2853-2861 https://doi.org/10.2527/jas.2007-0616

Wilsson E, Sundgren PE. The use of a behaviour test for selection of dogs for service and breeding. 2. Heritability for tested parameters and effect of selection based on service dog characteristics. Appl Anim Behav Sci. 1997; 54:(2–3)235-241 https://doi.org/10.1016/S0168-1591(96)01175-6

Yu Y, Wilson B, Masters S, van Rooy D, McGreevy PD. Mortality resulting from undesirable behaviours in dogs aged three years and under attending primary-care veterinary practices in Australia. Animals (Basel). 2021; 11:(2) https://doi.org/10.3390/ani11020493

Zapata I, Serpell JA, Alvarez CE. Genetic mapping of canine fear and aggression. BMC Genomics. 2016; 17 https://doi.org/10.1186/s12864-016-2936-3

Noise reactivity and firework fear in dogs

02 December 2023
8 mins read
Volume 28 · Issue 12

Abstract

Noise reactivity and firework fear are well-known and often discussed topics among dog owners. The prevalence of noise reactivity varies a lot between different dog breeds; in certain breeds, as many as 50% of all dogs are affected at some level. Even though the prevalence of noise reactivity is high, little is known about the genetic background of noise reactivity in dogs. This article provides a brief introduction to behavioural genetics and the genetics behind noise reactivity and firework fear, some of the animal welfare issues related to extreme fear of noise, and sheds light on the possibilities of reducing the prevalence through systematic breeding.

The fear of loud and sudden noises is a well-known problem for many companion and working dogs. Dogs may react to everyday noises such as the rattling of kitchen utensils or vacuum cleaners, traffic noises and construction work, and weather phenomena such as rain, thunder and wind. The most frequent fear reactions are reported to happen when the dogs are exposed to (unexpected) sudden and loud noises such as fireworks, gunshots, thunder and roaring traffic noises (Sherman and Mills, 2008; Storengen and Lingaas, 2015; Salonen et al, 2020). Commonly displayed fear-related behaviours include (Overall et al, 2001; Blackwell et al, 2013; Tiira et al, 2016; Handegård et al, 2020):

  • Trembling or shaking
  • Attention seeking
  • Hiding
  • Vocalising
  • Panting
  • Drooling or licking
  • Destructiveness
  • Restlessness
  • Indoor toileting
  • Self-mutilation
  • Refusal of food or water.

Owners of the most noise-reactive dogs report that the dogs may display these behaviours in the hours before events with large fireworks displays, such as on New Year's Eve (Handegård et al, 2020).

Understanding the genetic background of noise reactivity in dogs can help breeders make informed decisions to reduce the prevalence of noise-related behavioural issues in specific breeds. It can also shed light on behaviour modification strategies for affected dogs, potentially leading to more effective interventions. For example, knowledge of specific genetic markers associated with noise reactivity could enable targeted therapies or medications. However, genetic studies on noise reactivity are not straightforward.

Noise reactivity, like other behaviour traits, is complex. There is huge variation in phenotype presentation; dogs may be fearful of all, just one or a few very specific noises, and display a range of different behaviours, from a slight tremble to complete panic. Complex traits are affected by a combination of genetic factors, environmental factors and life experiences (Blackwell et al, 2013), where the importance and value of the individual effects are unknown and/or variable (Figure 1). For example, studies have found that factors such as gender, age, castration status, early experiences and the owner's gender may be important in the development of noise reactivity (Blackwell et al, 2013; Tiira and Lohi, 2015; Cannas et al, 2018; Salonen et al, 2022). It has also been suggested that some noise-reactive dogs suffer from musculoskeletal pain, pain in the ears and/or a change in auditory response (Scheifele et al, 2016; Tiira et al, 2016; Lopes Fagundes et al, 2018). A thorough clinical health examination is always advised in all dogs that display excessive fearfulness or any other problematic behaviour.

Figure 1. Fearfulness and other behaviour traits are complex and influenced by a number of different factors, both genetic and environmental, where the importance of the individual effects are unknown and variable.

Studies on fear, phobia and anxiety have been conducted in numerous species, both domestic and wild animals, as well as laboratory animals; however, no consistent definition of fearfulness as a behavioural trait exists in veterinary medicine (Mobbs et al, 2019). Different literature, as well as different veterinarians and behaviourists, uses terminology like ‘noise reactivity’, ‘noise aversion’, ‘noise sensitivity’, ‘noise phobia’, ‘noise anxiety’ and ‘fear response to noise’ to describe, at least partly, the same phenomenon of dogs showing abnormally strong reactions to all or specific noises (Stephens-Lewis et al, 2022).

Fear, phobia and anxiety have different definitions, but in everyday speech among dog owners and veterinarians, these terms are very often mixed and used interchangeably. Similarly, there is a spectrum of different approaches to behavioural assessment in dogs, including battery testing, owner-directed survey and expert breed assessment (Jones and Gosling, 2005; Spady and Ostrander, 2008). These tests are often adapted to different breeds, ages and purposes; for example, the ‘dog mentality assessment’, the ‘behaviour and personality assessment’ and the ‘public access test’. Special tests have been adapted for puppies with the purpose of selecting the best-qualified individuals for future training. The results of such tests may then be used to select breeding animals, potential working dogs or assess the heritability of different personality traits (Wilsson and Sundgren, 1997; Ruefenacht et al, 2002; Lazarowski et al, 2021). Likewise, different versions of questionnaires have been developed: the most frequently used to date is the canine behavioural assessment and research questionnaire (Hsu and Serpell, 2003). This lack of unified terminology and standardised, well-validated behaviour recording schemes makes it difficult to compare data between studies, thus making epidemiological studies of both genetic background and treatment of behavioural problems in dogs very challenging (Overall, 2013). The great majority of noise-reactive dogs never undergo a behaviour test, and relatively few owners seek professional help for noise-reactive dogs (Rugbjerg et al, 2003; Dale et al, 2010; Blackwell et al, 2013).

Genetic background of noise reactivity

Genetic mutations occur in different parts of the genome all the time, and cause genetic differences between individuals, populations and species. Some mutations may be beneficial and provide a selective advantage to individuals carrying them, which leads to the spread of those mutations in a population. When a particular beneficial mutation becomes fixed, every individual in the population possesses that mutation. In genetics, an allele is a variant form of a gene. When an allele becomes fixed in a population, every individual in that population has that allele for a specific gene. In wild animals, this typically occurs through the process of genetic drift or natural selection. In modern dog breeds, artificial selection of specific traits (beneficial or not) has created breed-specific fixed alleles, which makes it possible to distinguish one breed from another through DNA tests. Phenotypically, all dogs of the same breed have the same unique characteristics that identify the breed – they are observed as fixed traits. One of the key characteristics of a fixed trait is the absence of significant variation within a population. In other words, individuals within the population tend to have similar genetic makeup regarding that specific trait.

Fearfulness and other behaviour traits are likely highly polygenic, meaning they are controlled by a large number of gene alleles which all have small effects. It is also very likely that many of these alleles are fixed in the population. Noise reactivity occurs in all purebreeds and mixed breeds, which is no surprise given the fact that fear, including fear of noises, is a natural trait and an essential part of natural selection and survival skills (McFarland, 1981; Erhardt and Spoormaker, 2013; Ressler, 2020). Fearfulness, by nature, is a very useful trait. Fear makes animals (and humans) keep a safe distance from things that may be harmful, and thus is a trait that has been very beneficial for dogs for many generations before they became domesticated. In some cases, however, the fearfulness becomes more severe than ‘normal’ and may ultimately negatively impact the dog's welfare and the dog-owner relationship, and the behaviour may then be classified as pathological (Overall, 2013). The impact of extreme fearfulness in dogs can be huge, both for the individual dog and animals and humans it interacts with (Rugbjerg et al, 2003; Shore, 2005; Cannas et al, 2018), and may have a profound negative impact on the dog's life expectancy (Scarlett et al, 1999; Dreschel, 2010). While noise reactivity may not directly determine a dog's lifespan, the stress and anxiety associated with such traits could potentially contribute to health issues that may impact longevity. Additionally, dogs may be more prone to accidents or injuries if they display extreme fear reactions, for instance by running away from the owner. Dogs that are overly sensitive to noise may have a reduced quality of life if they are constantly distressed by their environment, and if the problem becomes impossible to manage the owner may opt for euthanasia or rehoming.

Studies show that there are significant breed differences in the prevalence and severity of noise reactivity. In some breeds, as many as 30% show a strong or very strong fear of loud noises, and more than 50% may show some signs of noise reactivity (Blackwell et al, 2013; Storengen and Lingaas, 2015; Tiira et al, 2016; Riemer, 2019; Handegård et al, 2020; Salonen et al, 2020). This difference between breeds suggests a relevant genetic component to noise reactivity (Morrow et al, 2015; Storengen and Lingaas, 2015; Overall et al, 2016) which may be explained by selective breeding; for example, gun shyness is not a favourable trait in hunting dogs, while guard and herding dogs may exhibit height-ened vigilance and reactivity as a result of their historical roles as guardians and protectors.

Many studies on behaviour have been conducted as heritability studies. Heritability is measured from 0 to 1, where 0 is low and 1 is high. Heritability estimates indicate how much of the phenotypical variation in a population can be explained by genetic variation or in other words: how much of the phenotypical variation cannot be explained by environmental factors or by chance. Thus, heritability must not be confused with heredity. Environmental factors are likely to play a big role in the development of behaviour problems, including excessive fear. However, heritability estimates of non-social or noise-related fear show that these behaviours also have a significant genetic component (Table 1). This indicates that it should be possible to reduce the prevalence of reactivity to loud noises through the systematic breeding of less fearful dogs.


Table 1. Heritability (narrow) for a selection of noise/fear-related behaviour traits in dogs
Trait Heritability (h2) Breed
Curiosity/fearlessness 0.23 German shepherd dog
Fear 0.46 Guide dogs (Labrador retriever)
Firework fear 0.16 Standard poodle
Gun shyness 0.56/0.21 Labrador retriever/German shepherd dog
Gunfire reaction 0.19/0.23 German shepherd dog
Gunshot fear 0.37 Flat coated retriever
Non-social fear 0.27 Golden retriever
(Goddard and Beilharz, 1983; Ruefenacht et al, 2002; Lindberg et al, 2004; Strandberg et al, 2005; van der Waaij et al, 2008; Meyer et al, 2012; Schiefelbein, 2012; Handegård et al, 2021)

The within-breed and between-breed variation in prevalence and presentation of noise reactivity, combined with low–medium heritability estimates, suggest the existence of causative genetic variants. Many studies have been performed in search of such variants but with limited success, probably because of the nature of complex traits. Genome-wide association studies have identified some candidate genes that may be of interest for several complex traits, including behaviour-related traits such as canine obsessive-compulsive disorder, sociability and fearfulness (Dodman et al, 2010; Tang et al, 2014; Persson et al, 2016; Zapata et al, 2016; Sarviaho et al, 2019; 2020; Shan et al, 2021). The development in gene technology is progressing fast, and genome-wide association studies are now being rapidly replaced by more modern methods such as whole genome sequencing, third-generation sequencing and low-pass sequencing. Almost everything that is known about the canine genome has been discovered in the past 20 years, and many more discoveries should be expected to unfold over the next decades.

Animal welfare

Fear, phobias and anxiety are negative emotions that may have a significant effect on the welfare of dogs (Sherman and Mills, 2008; Bovenkerk and Nijland, 2017). In selected breeds, around 50% of dog owners report that their dogs show some sign of fear when exposed to loud and sudden noises, such as fireworks (Blackwell et al, 2013; Handegård et al, 2020), which makes it one of the most extensive welfare issues for companion dogs. For 10–50% of relinquished dogs, the owner states that behaviour issues are part of the reason why they feel unable to keep the dog (Marston et al, 2004; Kwan and Bain, 2013; Salonen et al, 2020); most of these dogs are younger than 3 years old (Kwan and Bain, 2013). These findings are supported by two studies based on veterinary records which found that undesired behaviour was the predominant cause of mortality for dogs aged 3 years or younger (Boyd et al, 2018; Yu et al, 2021). Fear-related behaviour is also a common reason for dogs being released from guide dog training programmes, withdrawn after successful guide dog training (Batt et al, 2008; Caron-Lormier et al, 2016) and in discharged adult military dogs (Evans et al, 2007). A positive association has been found between noise reactivity and other serious behavioural problems, including aggression, separation anxiety and general fearfulness (Overall et al, 2001; Tiira et al, 2016; Salonen et al, 2020), which are in themselves well-known reasons for euthanasia and relinquishment. It has also been documented that many dog owners are unaware of the possibility of behaviour modification and pharmaceutical treatment of firework fear (Blackwell et al, 2013). Habituation, training and psychopharmaceutic intervention have well-documented effects on fearfulness in dogs, so owners should be encouraged to seek professional help and advice.

Conclusions

Noise reactivity is a profound problem for many dogs and the fear reactions are often severe. To date, little is known about the genetic influence on fear, or why specific noises such as fireworks cause extreme fear reactions in some dogs. It is difficult to claim that all dogs with some degree of noise reactivity have reduced animal welfare. However, with evidence that noise reactivity has a relevant hereditary component, it may be advisable to recommend that seriously affected dogs should not be used for breeding, which over time can help reduce the prevalence of noise reactivity in the breed. Continued research on canine fear and behaviour promises to improve understanding of noise reactivity, ultimately benefiting the welfare of dogs and enhancing the bond between humans and their canine companions.

KEY POINTS

  • Canine behaviours are complex traits.
  • Noise reactivity and firework fear have a genetic component.
  • Severe noise reactivity can pose a welfare issue in dogs.
  • Systematic breeding can reduce the prevalence of noise reactivity.